Publication Cover
Reading & Writing Quarterly
Overcoming Learning Difficulties
Volume 34, 2018 - Issue 5
3,106
Views
14
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Articles

Strengthening the Reader Self-Efficacies of Reluctant and Struggling Readers Through Literature Circles

ORCID Icon

Abstract

Although a subskills approach to reading instruction is merited in improving the reading skills of struggling readers, struggling readers also benefit from balanced literacy instruction. An overemphasis on reading subskills arguably minimizes students’ enjoyment of reading and motivation to read and may inevitably result in the formation of reluctant or struggling readers. In turn, this may diminish the self-efficacies of reluctant or struggling readers. I designed an embedded multiple case study framed by social cognitive theory to explore the potential influence of literature circles, a balanced literacy instructional strategy, on the reader self-efficacies of reluctant and struggling readers in Grades 4–6. In terms of reader self-efficacy, reluctant and struggling readers appeared to benefit more from participating in literature circles than their classmates (i.e., students with initially higher reader self-efficacies). This study is significant in providing support for a more balanced approach to reading instruction, particularly for reluctant and struggling readers.

Struggling readers are more likely than more proficient readers to receive teacher-centered, subskills-focused reading instruction (Allington, Citation1983). Instruction in reading subskills is merited and supported by research (Scammacca, Roberts, Vaughn, & Stuebing, Citation2015; Wanzek, Wexler, Vaughn, & Ciullo, Citation2010) yet too often is the predominant form of reading instruction received by struggling readers (Allington, Citation1983). Struggling readers with low self-efficacy often doubt that their reading skills can be improved (Cho et al., Citation2015). Thus, struggling readers require a more balanced approach to literacy instruction to develop, hone, and successfully apply their reading skills within the context of authentic literacy. In this article, I argue that literature circles—a balanced literacy instructional strategy—can potentially strengthen the reader self-efficacies of reluctant and struggling readers.

A meta-analysis conducted by Scammacca et al. (Citation2015) revealed that struggling readers in Grades 4–12 receive primarily skills-based reading intervention, which can improve reading comprehension, fluency, and vocabulary. Thus, there is evidentiary support that targeted skills-based intervention is beneficial for struggling readers in Grades 4–12 (Scammacca et al., Citation2015). A similar synthesis of reading interventions for Grade 4–5 students with reading difficulties and/or disabilities found that researcher-developed interventions were particularly successful in helping students to improve their reading comprehension (Wanzek et al., Citation2010). Although reading subskills are foundational to literacy success, they cannot be the sole focus of reading instruction, particularly as students enter Grade 4. Kim et al. (Citation2016) cautioned, “Improvements in reading subskills, however, are not sufficient for deep comprehension” (p. 358). Moreover, reading instruction begins to shift in Grades 4–6 from developing reading subskills to applying those skills to increasingly difficult texts (Kent, Wanzek, & Al Otaiba, Citation2017). Yet opportunities to partake in authentic literacy experiences may be absent when a subskills approach to reading instruction is overemphasized.

“Despite the fact that balanced reading instruction is supported by the International [Literacy] Association, some schools have shifted toward direct instruction characterized by teacher-centered reading approaches” (Thames et al., Citation2008, p. 89). This is particularly true for struggling students, whom Allington (Citation1983) found were provided more skills-focused reading instruction while their peers received comprehension-focused instruction and read authentic texts. Allington (Citation1983) argued, “Good and poor readers differ in their reading ability as much because of differences in instruction” (p. 548). Like their peers, struggling readers benefit from reading instruction that encourages critical thinking as well as engaging in listening, reading, writing, and dialoguing (Thames et al., Citation2008). Recent research conducted by Kim et al. (2016) studied the effect of Strategic Adolescent Reading Intervention, a supplemental reading program melding skills-based instruction with text-based discussion, on struggling readers in Grades 6–8. Students who received the Strategic Adolescent Reading Intervention outperformed the control group in terms of word recognition, morphological awareness, and reading comprehension (Kim et al., 2016). Improving reading achievement as well as fostering students’ enjoyment of reading and motivation to read warrants a more balanced approach to reading instruction (i.e., mediating a reading subskills approach with authentic opportunities to engage in reading, writing, and speaking).

Yet improving the reading achievement of reluctant and struggling readers necessitates improving their reader self-efficacies. Self-efficacy refers to an individual’s belief that he or she can succeed at a given task (Bandura, Citation1997). Often struggling readers have low reader self-efficacies (Ferrara, Citation2005). Thus, struggling readers must come to believe that they can read successfully by applying learned skills and strategies (McCabe & Margolis, Citation2001). Readers with low self-efficacies are unlikely to transfer their learning from the context in which they acquired their knowledge and skills to a different one (McCabe, Citation2003). Thus, self-efficacy can influence a struggling reader’s application of learned reading skills and strategies. Cho et al. (2015) found that when students received reading intervention, the reading outcomes of students with low reading comprehension yet higher self-efficacies were more likely to improve than those of students with lower self-efficacies.

For this study, I distinguish between reluctant readers and struggling readers. A reluctant reader is capable of reading (i.e., he or she has the skills to read) yet does not do so unless necessary for work or daily life (Chambers, Citation1969). A struggling reader often reads below grade level (Hoyte, Citation2006). The terms reluctant reader and struggling reader are often used synonymously. As Beers (Citation1996) explained, a reluctant reader (i.e., a person who does not enjoy reading) may evolve into a struggling reader because of a lack of reading practice. A lack of balanced literacy instruction can be detrimental to all students but particularly to reluctant and struggling readers. Thus, the purpose of this embedded multiple case study was to explore the potential influence of participating in literature circles on the reader self-efficacies of reluctant and struggling readers.

Review of the literature and theoretical framework

Literature circles fall within the realm of balanced literacy instruction. Carrison and Ernst-Slavit (Citation2005) explained, “The literature circle model, as a part of a balanced literacy program provides abundant opportunities for dynamic interaction among readers and text” (p. 110). A literature circle is a “small, peer-led discussion group whose members have chosen to read the same story, poem, article, or book” (Daniels, Citation2002, p. 2). Ultimately the goal of literacy instruction is to equip students to read authentic literature (Cooper, Robinson, Slansky, & Kiger, Citation2015). Authentic literature refers to trade books or narrative and expository texts in their original forms (Cooper et al., Citation2015) and contrasts skills-heavy basals readers created specifically to teach reading subskills. As a literature circle participant, each student reads authentic literature while honing his or her literacy skills. In preparation for literature circles, a student may engage in one or more of the following processes: (a) making predictions, (b) identifying important information, (c) making inferences, (d) monitoring comprehension, (e) seeking clarification, (f) asking and answering questions, (g) visualizing, and (h) summarizing (Whittaker, Citation2012). Each of these processes is a research-based strategy for constructing meaning (Cooper et al., Citation2015).

Research attests to the potential benefits of literature circles. For example, participating in a literature circle promotes higher order thinking through student-centered instruction (Cameron, Murray, Hull, & Cameron, Citation2012). Text-based discussions, such as those at the heart of literature circles, help students to develop reading strategies, improve prosody, increase their stamina and perseverance, and develop critical literacy skills (Pearson, Citation2010). Participation in a literature circle improves reading comprehension (Clarke & Holwadel, Citation2007) and, related to this, benefits students with low reading comprehension (Avci & Yuksel, Citation2011). Literature circles increase students’ motivation to read (Stein & Beed, Citation2004) and improve reluctant readers’ enjoyment of reading (Cameron et al., Citation2012). Therefore, literature circles are promising for reluctant and struggling readers.

Some commonalities between the present study and a study conducted by Hall (Citation2012) exist, including a focus on struggling readers, the instrument used to measure reader self-efficacy, and the utilization of text-based discussions. Hall utilized the Reader Self-Perception Scale (RSPS; Henk & Melnick, Citation1995) as one data source in her study of struggling readers’ interactions in text-based discussion groups. Hall found that struggling readers were initially reserved in their text-based discussion groups yet began to contribute more over the course of their group meetings. The present study, however, differs from Hall in its focus on reader self-efficacy and the inclusion of reluctant readers as participants. Thus, this study is unique in relation to research on both self-efficacy and literature circles.

I approached this study through the lens of social cognitive theory, fathered by Albert Bandura. Bandura (Citation2001) postulated that humans are cognitive agents influenced by their own thoughts and beliefs, including self-efficacy. This study—focused on the influence of participating in literature circles on the reader self-efficacies of reluctant and struggling readers in an intermediate grades classroom—was framed by social cognitive theory.

Self-efficacy is defined as confidence in one’s “ability to organize and execute a given course of action to solve a problem or accomplish a task” (Bandura, Citation1997, p. 3). Self-efficacy is task specific (McCabe, Citation2003). Thus, a student may have higher self-efficacy in one content area (e.g., mathematics) but lower self-efficacy in another (e.g., English language arts and reading). Four sources of information compose self-efficacy: (a) mastery experience, (b) vicarious experience, (c) feedback, and (d) physiological states (Pajares, Citation2003). One’s self-efficacy influences task engagement, the amount of effort put forth toward accomplishing the task, and persistence in the face of difficulty (Bandura & Schunk, Citation1981). Therefore, literacy educators must understand the role of self-efficacy in learning.

An individual’s cognitive ability as well as self-efficacy influence his or her literacy development (Corkett, Hatt, & Benevides, Citation2011). Furthermore, self-efficacy as a motivational factor can likewise influence literacy development (Schunk & Zimmerman, Citation2007). Readers with low self-efficacies may avoid or withdraw from tasks perceived as too difficult (Guthrie et al., Citation2007; Zimmerman, Citation2000). Consequently, readers with low self-efficacies may miss opportunities to improve their reading comprehension (Solheim, Citation2011). Pajares (Citation2003) cautioned that unwarranted low self-efficacy, rather than ability, can diminish a student’s academic achievement. Furthermore, recent research underscores the connection between self-efficacy and literacy development/achievement (Corkett et al., Citation2011; J. K. Smith, Smith, Gilmore, & Jameson, Citation2012; Solheim, Citation2011).

Research suggests that self-efficacy influences reading enjoyment, reading comprehension, and literacy-related achievement. J. K. Smith et al. (Citation2012) found a correlation between reading self-efficacy and reading enjoyment. The researchers found that Grade 4 and Grade 8 students with higher reading self-efficacies viewed reading as more enjoyable than their peers with lower reading self-efficacies (J. K. Smith et al., Citation2012). Solheim (Citation2011) found a correlation between reading self-efficacy and reading comprehension. Grade 5 students with high reading self-efficacies performed well on both multiple-choice and created-response reading comprehension questions (Solheim, Citation2011). Readers with low self-efficacies, however, performed better on created-response questions than multiple-choice questions (Solheim, Citation2011). Solheim’s research underscores the influence of a student’s self-efficacy on measures of reading achievement. In an era of high-stakes testing, such findings denote the importance of improving students’ self-efficacies to improve their reading comprehension and aptitude for multiple-choice standardized tests. Corkett et al. (Citation2011) also found a connection between students’ literacy (i.e., reading and writing) achievement and their self-efficacies. In their study, teachers’ perceptions of their students’ reading self-efficacies correlated with their students’ reading and writing abilities (Corkett et al., Citation2011). Literature circles—an engaging, student-centered instructional strategy utilizing authentic literature—can foster reading enjoyment, comprehension, and achievement.

There is a need to study the reader self-efficacies of Grade 4–6 students on two fronts. First, previous research suggests that Grade 4–6 students often fail to appraise accurately their reading self-efficacies (Corkett et al., Citation2011; Linnenbrink & Pintrich, Citation2003; Pajares, Citation2003; J. K. Smith et al., Citation2012). This is an issue of calibration, or the notion that one’s perceived self-efficacy is an accurate reflection of one’s actual performance (Linnenbrink & Pintrich, Citation2003). The ability to calibrate accurately one’s self-efficacy with one’s ability improves with age (Shell, Colvin, & Bruning, Citation1995). However, low self-efficacy can supersede skill (Bandura, Citation1997), which is potentially dangerous for readers in Grades 4–6. Because reading motivation is known to decline in the adolescent years (Unrau & Schlackman, Citation2006), educators must intentionally build the reader self-efficacies of Grade 4–6 students who are transitioning into adolescence. Second, the onus is on Grade 4–6 teachers to cultivate their students’ reader self-efficacies. Teachers in a study conducted by Corkett et al. (Citation2011) were unaware of the role literacy-related self-efficacy played in their students’ achievement. However, teachers need to be intentional in strengthening their students’ reading self-efficacies. Schunk and Zimmerman (Citation2007) recommended that teachers build students’ self-efficacies through encouraging feedback as well as opportunities to gain mastery experiences. For example, teachers can utilize cooperative learning opportunities to build a student’s self-efficacy through vicarious experience (Schunk & Zimmerman, Citation2007). Cooperative learning enhances vicarious experience, as a student will likely identify with at least one model in the group (Schunk, Citation1987). The focus on self-efficacy within the context of cooperative learning via literature circles led me to frame this study from the perspective of social cognitive theory.

Methodology

Purpose of the study

The purpose of this embedded multiple case study was to explore the influence of participation in a literature circle on the reader self-efficacies of reluctant and struggling readers in a Grade 4–6 classroom. This study was primarily qualitative with quantitative data collection embedded to enhance its robustness. An embedded case study integrates quantitative data into a primarily qualitative study (Yin, Citation2014). A common justification for utilizing both qualitative and quantitative data is to facilitate triangulation, specifically in “seeking corroboration between quantitative and qualitative data” (Bryman, Citation2006, p. 105). The use of multiple cases in this study further illustrated the issue (Creswell, Citation2013) of the reader self-efficacies of reluctant and struggling readers. Herriott and Firestone (Citation1983) asserted that multiple case studies are more robust because data from several cases inform generalizations made from collected data. Quantitative data—collected via the RSPS (Henk & Melnick, Citation1995)—served to supplement the findings from qualitative data. The design of an embedded multiple case study was selected to best suit the research question.

Research question

The research question at the center of this study asked the following: How does participating in a literature circle influence the reader self-efficacies of reluctant and struggling readers? Prior to collecting data, I predicted that engaging in literature-based conversations would positively influence, or strengthen, the reader self-efficacies of the reluctant and struggling readers who participated in this study.

Site of this study

The site of this study was a Grade 4–6 classroom within a central Texas public school. I elected to study reluctant and struggling readers in these grades because negative attitudes toward reading often begin in these formative years (Brinda, Citation2011). Data collection occurred in the classroom of Mrs. Ian (a pseudonym). The school at which this study was conducted was selected because it was fairly representative of public schools within the region.

Sampling procedure

All 20 students in Mrs. Ian’s classroom participated in literature circles. However, only 17 students participated in this study. The parents of one student did not consent to her participation in this research study; the data of another student were incomplete and could not be utilized. One student receiving special education services also did not participate. Although 17 students participated in this study, only seven were identified as cases for further study. Criterion-based purposeful sampling was used to identify each case. Palinkas et al. (Citation2013) described criterion-based purposeful sampling as “identify[ing] and select[ing] all cases that meet some predetermined criterion of importance” (p. 535). The criteria for being identified as a case in this study were having a comparatively low reader self-efficacy score as self-reported via the RSPS (Henk & Melnick, Citation1995) as well as being identified as a reluctant or struggling reader by Mrs. Ian.

In September 2016, I supervised Mrs. Ian’s students as they independently completed the RSPS (Henk & Melnick, Citation1995) pretest. I then scored the pretests and rank-ordered the students from the highest RSPS pretest score (i.e., highest self-reported reader self-efficacy) to the lowest RSPS pretest score (i.e., lowest self-reported reader self-efficacy). Subsequently, I identified the seven students with the lowest pretest scores, or lowest self-reported reader self-efficacies, as potential cases. I then asked Mrs. Ian to identify each potential case as a reluctant reader, a struggling reader, or neither (thereby excluding him or her as a case). Mrs. Ian’s professional judgment served to identify potential cases because the 2016–2017 school year was the second or third year that Mrs. Ian had taught many of her students (i.e., those in Grades 5 and 6). A meta-analysis of 75 studies found that “the correlation between teachers’ judgements of students’ academic achievement … is positive and fairly high (.63)” (Südkamp, Kaiser, & Möller, Citation2012, p. 755). Quantitative data, via the RSPS pre- and posttests, were collected from each study participant. However, qualitative data were collected only for the seven students who met the aforementioned criteria and thereby served as cases in this study.

Cases

The cases in this study were representative of the larger school population—six out of the seven were students of color. Two cases represented each grade level, with the exception of Grade 5, from which three cases participated. Mrs. Ian identified three of the cases as reluctant readers, two as struggling readers, and two as both reluctant and struggling readers. shows the demographic data for each case.

Table 1. Case demographics.

Literature circles in Mrs. Ian’s classroom

As there is no uniform manner in which to implement literature circles, it is important to contextualize those within Mrs. Ian’s classroom. Literature circles are essentially book clubs structured for school settings. Although there are a few defining characteristics (i.e., small groups discussing the same text), educators have a great amount of freedom in how they structure literature circles. The literature circles observed for this study occurred over an 8-week period from November 2016 to January 2017 (excluding holiday breaks). The students voted to read the same novel: Rules by Cynthia Lord (Citation2008). The seven cases (i.e., students identified as reluctant and struggling readers) and 10 other study participants (i.e., students with higher reader self-efficacies) intermingled within literature circles. Both quantitative and qualitative data were collected for this embedded multiple case study prior to, during, and following the 8-week literature circle cycle.

Data collection

To allow for triangulation, I collected data from a variety of sources, including (a) RSPS pre- and posttest scores, (b) individual interviews with the cases and classroom teacher, (c) direct observations, (d) documentation (i.e., literature circle role sheets), and (e) a focus group. The use of multiple data sources facilitates triangulation to corroborate evidence (Lincoln & Guba, Citation1985) and provides validity to research findings (Creswell, Citation2013).

The RSPS

I utilized the RSPS (Henk & Melnick, Citation1995) because it is an instrument created specifically to measure the reader self-efficacies of Grade 4–6 students (Henk & Melnick, Citation1992). Four scales make up the RSPS: (a) Progress, a measure of the student’s perceived reading performance in comparison with his or her past performance; (b) Observational Comparison, which measures a student’s perceived reading performance relative to that of his or her peers; (c) Social Feedback, in which the student reflects on input from his or her teachers, classmates, and family; and (d) Physiological States, a measure of the internal feelings experienced by the student while reading (Henk & Melnick, Citation1995). Each subscale of the RSPS (Henk & Melnick, Citation1995) corresponds to one of four factors composing self-efficacy. The statement “I think I am a good reader” is included on the RSPS as a measure of general reader self-efficacy. More than 2,100 Grade 4–6 students across a variety of school contexts (i.e., rural, suburban, and urban) served in norming the RSPS, which is both reliable (αs = .81–.84) and valid (Henk & Melnick, Citation1995). Results from pretest and posttest administrations of the RSPS made up the quantitative strand of data collected for this study and allowed me to gauge quantitatively the influence of literature circles on participants’ reader self-efficacies. As mentioned previously, the RSPS was self-administered by all study participants as a pretest in September 2016 prior to the observed literature circle cycle. The RSPS was again self-administered by all study participants as a posttest in February 2017 following the conclusion of the observed literature circle cycle. Additional qualitative data were collected on the seven cases who merited further study.

Interviews

I conducted an individual interview with each case in October 2016 prior to his or her participation in the observed literature circles. In February 2017, after the conclusion of the literature circle cycle, I individually interviewed each case once again. All interviews were audiotaped to ensure that all responses were accurately recorded, as suggested by Yin (2014). I likewise interviewed Mrs. Ian following the 8-week literature circle cycle, as she was instrumental in helping me to identify the seven cases for further study. During this interview, Mrs. Ian was asked to reflect on the 8-week literature circle cycle as well as the cases as readers prior to and following participation in literature circles.

Direct observations

Observation involves coupling the observer’s senses with an instrument to note a phenomenon within the field (Angrosino, Citation2007). Each week from November 2016 to January 2017 I visited each literature circle as it met to discuss Rules (Lord, Citation2008). During these observations, I assumed the role of participant observer to allow for full interaction with the cases (Creswell, Citation2013). A checklist developed by Day, Spiegel, McLellan, and Brown (Citation2002) coupled with an open-ended observational protocol that I developed were used to note the interactions of the cases with others.

Documentation

For each literature circle meeting, Mrs. Ian assigned each student a role and expected him or her to read the text through the lens of the assigned role. The respective role assigned to each participant changed from week to week. The roles were as follows: (a) Artful Artist—illustrates characters and/or pivotal scenes in the text; (b) Connector—makes text-to-self, text-to-text, and text-to-world connections; (c) Discussion Director—guides discussion through questioning; (d) Literacy Luminary—identifies memorable and/or important passages in the text; (e) Summarizer—summarizes the chapter(s) read; and (f) Word Wizard—identifies interesting and/or puzzling words encountered in the text. For each literature circle meeting, each participant prepared a role sheet. I analyzed only those role sheets prepared by the seven cases, which served as documentation in this study.

Focus group

Following the 8-week literature circle cycle, I conducted a focus group with six of the seven cases. The purpose of the focus group was to gain further insight into the cases’ experiences in literature circles as well as any influence literature circles had on them as readers.

Data analysis

Qualitative data analysis occurred via the data analysis spiral (Creswell, Citation2013). After organizing the data using NVivo 11, I read the qualitative data in their entirety prior to memoing, described by Creswell (Citation2013) as taking note of “short phrases, ideas, or key concepts that occur to the reader” (p. 183). After memoing, I categorized the data before coding them into themes, or common ideas resulting from the aggregation of several codes, as recommended by Creswell.

To conduct within-case analysis, I engaged in pattern matching, described by Yin (2014) as comparing “an empirically based pattern” (p. 143) that evolves from the data to a pattern predicted prior to collecting said data. As stated previously, I predicted that participating in a literature circle would positively influence the reader self-efficacies of the reluctant and struggling readers who served as cases in this study. As a multiple case study, this study also necessitated cross-case analysis. Yin recommended treating each case as a separate study. Thus, I engaged in the data analysis spiral for each of the seven cases.

To analyze the quantitative data, I used a dependent (paired) samples t test to determine whether the cases’ reader self-efficacies, as measured by the RSPS, changed following their participation in literature circles. A dependent (paired) samples t test was used to analyze the data because the same cases were tested twice using the RSPS, as suggested by Kirk (Citation2008). I also utilized a dependent (paired) samples t test to analyze the RSPS pre- and posttest scores of the other study participants who did not serve as cases (i.e., the students with higher reader self-efficacies). Described below are the results and findings from both the qualitative and quantitative data analyses.

Results and findings

The self-reported reader self-efficacies of the reluctant and struggling readers who served as cases in this study improved between the RSPS pre- and posttests. These results corroborate findings from qualitative data. Yet the influence of participation in the literature circle on the reader self-efficacies of the other study participants (i.e., the students with higher reader self-efficacies) was not as pronounced.

Seven cases: Reluctant and struggling readers

As measured by the RSPS (Henk & Melnick, Citation1995), the self-reported reader self-efficacies of the reluctant and struggling readers who served as cases in this study improved from pretest to posttest. summarizes the results from the quantitative analysis. Results from the dependent (paired) samples t test indicated a statistically significant difference between cases’ RSPS pretest scores (M = 2.99, SD = 0.50) and their posttest scores (M = 3.69, SD = 0.45), t(6) = −3.76, p = .009, d = 1.41. The effect size for this analysis (d = 1.41) was found to exceed Cohen’s (Citation1988) convention for a large effect (d = 0.80). These statistically significant results suggest that the self-reported reader self-efficacies of reluctant and struggling readers increased from pretest to posttest. In addition to the collective improvement in the reader self-efficacies of the cases, the reader self-efficacy of each case improved following his or her participation in literature circles. The mean RSPS pre- and posttest scores for each case as well as his or her pre- and posttest scores for each scale are found in . Overall, the reader self-efficacies of the seven reluctant and struggling readers who served as cases in this study improved, as demonstrated by gains in their RSPS scores from pretest to posttest and as supported by qualitative data.

Table 2. Results of a dependent-samples t test for cases’ RSPS scores.

Table 3. Cases’ RSPS pre- and posttest scores.

Grace

Prior to the literature circles, Grace described herself as a reader by remarking, “I’m a good reader depending on what time of day it is,” adding that she read best in the afternoon when she was neither sleepy nor grumpy (personal communication, October 2016). On her final role sheet, Grace wrote, “I also injoy [enjoy] doing it like this [reading the novel in a literature circle] because it gets stuff done” (documentation, January 2017). In an interview following the conclusion of the 8-week literature circle cycle, Grace expressed her enjoyment of reading by stating, “Yes … because I found interesting books that I like” (personal communication, February 2017). I asked Grace what had led to the improvement between her RSPS pre- and posttest scores. Grace explicitly attributed her improved scores to “the literature circle books … because I actually read it and I enjoyed it and I wrote about [it] too” (personal communication, February 2017). As in the first interview, Grace described herself as a “good reader” (personal communication, February 2017). This time, however, Grace offered a specific example—“I can actually find words, and I know what the words mean” (personal communication, February 2017)—to support her assertion and did not attribute her reading ability to a particular time of day. Reflecting on her literature circle experience, Grace remarked, “People in my group [literature circle] helped me understand the words too” (personal communication, February 2017). Furthermore, Grace stated, “It [literature circles] changed me to read more and like the books I read” (personal communication, February 2017). During the focus group, Grace stated that the literature circle “takes up time, but I liked it because you know, I didn’t have to do work a lot” (focus group, February 2017). I asked the cases to consider whether participating in a literature circle changed their perceptions of themselves as students. Grace responded, “Well, yeah, ‘cause I like to read now” (focus group, February 2017).

Humberto

When asked whether he enjoyed reading during the initial interview, Humberto responded, “[Reading] sometimes takes time like away from fun things” (personal communication, October 2016). Humberto admitted that reading was challenging for him. “Sometimes it’s hard to think about the reading, and I sometimes don’t know words that are being used. And it’s really hard to understand what a question means” (personal communication, October 2016). On his final role sheet, Humberto wrote, “I enjoy the book in groups [i.e., literature circles] because it’s much easier to learn about the book” (documentation, January 2017). Like Grace, Humberto credited differences between his RSPS pre- and posttest scores to his participation in literature circles. In the post–literature circle interview, Humberto was asked what had led to the improvement between his initial RSPS scores and posttest scores, to which he responded, “When we were doing literature circles” (personal communication, February 2017). In the same interview, Humberto stated that he liked reading “a little bit” but added that some books were too long for his taste (personal communication, February 2017). Note that Humberto stated that “defining words” (personal communication, February 2017) was one of his strengths as a reader, although prior to participating in literature circles he voiced that vocabulary was challenging for him. Humberto’s attitude toward reading seemed to improve minutely after he participated in literature circles because he stated that he would still rather “do something else instead of reading” (personal communication, February 2017). During the focus group, I asked the cases whether literature circles differed from the usual reading instruction they received. Humberto responded, “I mean, like, yeah. It’s [literature circles] less boring, and we have more conversations” (focus group, February 2017).

Lucita

Prior to participating in literature circles, I asked Lucita whether she liked to read, to which she responded, “No. Not a lot” (personal communication, October 2016). In reflecting on Lucita as a reader prior to literature circles, Mrs. Ian commented, “[Lucita] masked it [i.e., struggling with reading] very well, very well. She would smile and say she has it when she didn’t” (personal communication, February 2017). In the post–literature circle interview, I again asked Lucita whether she enjoyed reading. Lucita responded, “Well, since last time [i.e., October 2016], I kind of am starting to get used to it and liking it” (personal communication, February 2017). Lucita attributed her newfound affinity for reading to being able to visualize the text and concentrate on words, adding, “I’m starting to learn bigger words” (personal communication, February 2017). Lucita stated, “It’s [literature circles] helping me understand the reading” (personal communication, February 2017). Lucita ended our post–literature circle interview by commenting, “I improved … I’m getting smarter—growing more” (personal communication, February 2017). Mrs. Ian likewise noted Lucita’s improvement as a reader, stating, “It [literature circles] gave her an avenue to begin practicing skills she’s learning, and I think for her, it helped cement some of that understanding. I think it’s been helping her improve. I think it’s been helping her a lot” (personal communication, February 2017).

Naomi

I asked Naomi during our initial interview whether she enjoyed reading. Naomi responded, “Not really,” and when pressed by me retorted, “‘Cause I don’t know; it’s just I don’t like to read” (personal communication, October 2016). Furthermore, Naomi stated that she read only when “Mrs. Ian tells me to” (personal communication, October 2016). On reflecting on Naomi as a reader prior to literature circles, Mrs. Ian characterized her as a reluctant reader, stating, “Naomi is a strong reader, just does not always want to read. She’s … highly capable; she’s just very reluctant” (personal communication, February 2017). On her final role sheet Naomi wrote, “If I had another chance to do another lit meeting I would only [emphasis hers] if the book had pictures” (documentation, January 2017). During the interview following the 8-week literature circle cycle, Naomi described her literature circle experience in the following manner: “It wasn’t as bad as I thought it was going to be. I thought it was going to be like very boring and stuff, but it wasn’t that boring” (personal communication, February 2017). Naomi described herself as an “intelligent reader,” adding, “‘cause I can say words that most kids can’t, and I can like read better—like if the word is really long, I could probably figure it out” (personal communication, February 2017). Naomi attributed differences between her RSPS pre- and posttest scores to “reading like longer books instead of reading like shorter books” (personal communication, February 2017).

Tomás

Tomás stated during our initial interview that he enjoyed reading, adding, “It’s better for you … at school” (personal communication, October 2016). Tomás described himself as an “okay reader” (personal communication, October 2016), admitting that he struggled with vocabulary and needed help at times. Reflecting on Tomás as a reader prior to his participation in literature circles, Mrs. Ian characterized Tomás as both a reluctant and a struggling reader. Mrs. Ian stated, “I think his [Tomás’s] behaviors were getting in the way because he was basically doing anything he could to avoid participating in anything dealing with reading” (personal communication, February 2017). During the post–literature circle interview, Tomás stated, “I think reading’s fun and interesting” (personal communication, February 2017). After participating in literature circles, Tomás described himself as a “good” reader but added that he still “struggle[s] sometimes with words” (personal communication, February 2017). I asked Tomás about the improvement between his RSPS pretest and posttest scores. Tomás responded, “Because last time I took it [i.e., RSPS pretest], I was struggling with words more than when I took it again [i.e., RSPS posttest]. I was kind of not getting stuck” (personal communication, February 2017). Later in the same interview, Tomás was asked whether literature circles had changed him as a reader, to which he answered, “Some words I didn’t understand, but I took the book home and I read it overnight and I brought it back to school and so for our literature circle, I know what the words mean” (personal communication, February 2017). Mrs. Ian—who had been Tomás’s teacher the previous 2 years—noted that Tomás’s demeanor toward reading changed after he participated in literature circles. Mrs. Ian commented, “He [Tomás] wants to read, which is very different … from the fourth grader I started off with or even the fifth grader I had” (personal communication, February 2017). Mrs. Ian added that while in Grades 4 and 5, Tomás “would just kind of fall out in the middle of the floor and drag his feet and do everything he could to avoid it [reading]” (personal communication, February 2017). Furthermore, Mrs. Ian stated,

During literature circles, I saw several changes with him [Tomás]. He’s already asked to borrow two or three books out of my teacher library. Now he asks me, “Can I go outside and read?” Like he wants to read, which is very different. (personal communication, February 2017)

Will

In our initial interview, Will responded to a question about whether he enjoyed reading by stating, “Sometimes I don’t want to read. Sometimes I kind of don’t” (personal communication, October 2016). Mrs. Ian said,

I think when he [Will] is presented with more challenging reading then he draws back. He is reading on grade level according to every reading inventory I have, but I figured out it’s not “I can’t,” it’s “I don’t want to.” (personal communication, February 2017)

During the third week of literature circles, I overheard Will telling Mrs. Ian, “I hate reading” (observation, December 2016). Similarly, in Week 4, the student functioning in the role of Discussion Director posed a question regarding the group members’ least favorite parts of the book. Will retorted, “Reading. Because I hate reading” (observation, December 2016). Throughout the fourth literature circle meeting, Will continually answered, “I hate reading” (observation, December 2016) regardless of the question posed. However, Will’s attitude toward both participation in a literature circle and reading seemingly changed halfway through the literature circles. I did not overhear Will express his hatred of reading beginning in Week 5 or in the remaining weeks. On his final role sheet, Will reflected on his literature circle experience by characterizing literature circles as “sometimes boring and sometimes good and funny” (documentation, January 2017). During the second (i.e., post–literature circle) interview, Will offered this response to a question regarding his enjoyment of reading: “Yes because I like to read comics, and no, because sometimes I don’t like to read” (personal communication, February 2017). During the focus group, I asked the cases whether participating in a literature circle had led them to want to read more, less, or the same as before. It is striking that Will answered, “I like reading” (personal communication, February 2017). Mrs. Ian noted some changes in Will’s demeanor toward reading following his participation in literature circles. Mrs. Ian commented,

I think it [literature circles] made a little bit of a difference because even though he [Will] would kind of just say “I still don’t want to.” He still does it. And that I found interesting because before I wouldn’t have even been able to get him to even want to and then do it. He would say, “I don’t want to” and then not. But now I saw where he was actually putting forth the effort to do it and so that made a huge difference for him. (personal communication, February 2017)

Ximena

Prior to literature circles, Ximena expressed to me her enjoyment of reading. Ximena stated, “I just like to find out what kind of stories are there and … I just like to read” (personal communication, October 2016). Ximena admitted that reading could be challenging for her and stated, “Sometimes I mess up the words” (personal communication, October 2016). Mrs. Ian offered this characterization of Ximena: “I saw her as more of a struggling reader” (personal communication, February 2017). On her final role sheet, Ximena wrote, “It [literature circles] was good. I liked it” (documentation, January 2017). During our second interview, Ximena expressed that she liked literature circles because “I get to read and I get to write what is funny and what’s the problem” (personal communication, February 2017). Like Grace and Humberto, Ximena attributed improvement in her reader self-efficacy to participating in literature circles. When asked about differences between her RSPS pre- and posttest scores, Ximena offered the following explanation for her improvement: “Because I wasn’t a very good reader until I read and read every single day for our literature circle, and it made me a better reader” (personal communication, February 2017). Furthermore, Ximena stated that participating in literature circles helped her to “think of me as smart—a good reader” (personal communication, February 2017). Mrs. Ian noted changes in Ximena following the literature circle cycle: “Literature circles has helped kind of add to that confidence level and to use what she’s [Ximena’s] read and use what she’s learning on a deeper level” (personal communication, February 2017).

The reader self-efficacies of each reluctant reader and struggling reader who served as a case in this study improved as measured by the RSPS (Henk & Melnick, Citation1995) and as supported by qualitative data. Thus, the results and findings of this study suggest that literature circles can be influential in strengthening the reader self-efficacies of reluctant and struggling readers. Yet participating in a literature circle did not produce similar results in the other study participants.

Other study participants

When I examined the quantitative data for Mrs. Ian’s class, one surprising finding was that participation in literature circles did not yield positive change in the self-reported reader self-efficacies of the other study participants. synthesizes the results of the dependent (paired) samples t test for the other study participants, which decreased slightly (by 0.25 points) between pre- and posttest. These results contrasted those of the seven cases. Within this embedded multiple case study, qualitative data were collected only from the reluctant and struggling readers who served as cases because they were the primary focus of this study and met the sampling criteria. Because the other study participants (i.e., the students with initially higher reader self-efficacies) were not the intended focus of this study, I did not collect qualitative data from these students.

Table 4. Results of a dependent-samples t test for other study participants’ RSPS scores.

Discussion

The results and findings of this study suggest that participating in literature circles could potentially improve the reader self-efficacies of reluctant and struggling readers. The reader self-efficacies of three reluctant readers (i.e., Grace, Humberto, and Naomi), two struggling readers (i.e., Lucita and Ximena), and two students identified as both reluctant and struggling readers (i.e., Tomás and Will) improved as demonstrated by an increase in their RSPS scores from pre- to posttest. Moreover, findings from the qualitative data analysis suggest that participating in literature circles positively influenced each of the seven cases. Four of the seven cases (i.e., Grace, Humberto, Tomás, and Ximena) explicitly stated that participating in literature circles led to improvements between their RSPS pre- and posttest scores. Mrs. Ian vocalized improvement in four of the seven cases (i.e., Lucita, Tomás, Will, and Ximena) in their demeanors toward reading and/or reading skills. Five of the seven cases identified at least one strength in terms of their reading ability following their participation in literature circles. More specifically, Grace, Humberto, Lucita, and Naomi explicitly mentioned their abilities to problem-solve unknown words as a strength, whereas Ximena expressed an overall improvement in herself as a reader.

Educators must be intentional in seeking to strengthen the reader self-efficacies of reluctant and struggling readers. I designed this embedded multiple case study to explore the potential influence of participating in literature circles on the reader self-efficacies of reluctant and struggling readers in Grades 4–6. Prior to conducting this study, I predicted that the reader self-efficacies of the seven reluctant and struggling readers who served as cases in this study would be positively influenced, or strengthened, following their participation in literature circles. Social cognitive theory both informed this prediction and framed this study. Vicarious experience, or observing others’ success in a given task, is one source that informs self-efficacy. Within the context of this study, I postulated that cases’ self-efficacies could potentially be strengthened as they observed their peers demonstrating success within literature circles. Participation in literature circles could potentially provide cases with the opportunity to garner mastery experiences with reading as well as solicit feedback from their literature circle members, both of which could likewise inform self-efficacy. This study is significant because, as mentioned previously, the reader self-efficacies of Grade 4–6 students often do not correlate with their reading abilities. Therefore, there is a need to strengthen the reader self-efficacies of reluctant and struggling readers to improve their affinities for reading and arguably their reading achievement.

Although some may suggest that the RSPS scores of the cases increased from pre- to posttest because of the students’ natural growth over the course of the school year, I argue against this notion. As mentioned in the review of related literature, there is a lack of calibration between the reading abilities and the reading self-efficacies of Grade 4–6 students (Corkett et al., Citation2011; Linnenbrink & Pintrich, Citation2003; Pajares, Citation2003; J. K. Smith et al., Citation2012). Thus, although the reading ability of cases may have improved over the duration of the school year and course of this study, prior research suggests that improved reading ability does not necessarily translate into improved reader self-efficacy. Furthermore, outside of literature circles, students read only short passages focused on improving their reading subskills in preparation for the annual state standardized exam. An interview with Mrs. Ian attested to this notion, as she stated, “So much of what we read in class and even on our STAAR [i.e., State of Texas Assessments of Academic Readiness] test is very quick, short reads” (personal communication, February 2017). Reading short passages designed to improve reading subskills and/or for test preparation is unlikely to contribute to long-term reading growth. As Cabral-Marquez (Citation2015) explained, “It is only through sustained, active engagement with text that students will encounter natural, genuine opportunities to integrate all the skills and strategies that comprise the reading process and lead to growth in reading” (p. 464). Thus, I contend that the authentic literature read for literature circles had more of an influence on the reader self-efficacies of cases than the more frequently utilized shorter, subskills-focused texts. Comments from the cases and their classroom teacher suggest the same. Finally, higher self-efficacy is unlikely to be sustained if students are unsuccessful in a given task (Schunk & Zimmerman, Citation2007). Therefore, the cases’ elevated reader self-efficacies were more likely due to their successful participation in literature circles, which was the only changed variable in the type of reading instruction they received from Mrs. Ian over the course of the school year. Furthermore, participating in literature circles did not have the same influence on the reader self-efficacies of the other study participants, whose reader self-efficacies were initially higher. The incongruence in RSPS posttest results for cases and other study participants further suggests that participating in literature circles can potentially strengthen the reader self-efficacies of reluctant and struggling readers. As mentioned previously, both cases and other study participants intermingled in literature circles as well as read and discussed the same novel. In addition, Mrs. Ian changed the membership of each literature circle from week to week. Because both cases (i.e., reluctant and struggling readers with initially lower reader self-efficacies) and other study participants (i.e., students with initially higher reader self-efficacies) were subjected to the same aforementioned conditions yet produced different results on their RSPS posttests, the plausibility that participation in the literature circles increased the reader self-efficacies of cases is merited.

Connection to prior research

The results and findings of this study contribute to existing research on self-efficacy as well as literature circles/text-based discussions. Results and findings of this study corroborate those from previous research that suggested that participation in a literature circle can increase the confidence of young readers (Blum, Lipsett, & Yocom, Citation2002; Lewis, Citation1997). For example, Mrs. Ian noted that the confidence of Ximena—whom she had previously identified as a struggling reader—as a reader improved after the student’s participation in literature circles. Likewise, results and findings from this study support those of previous studies that suggested that literature circles increase students’ motivation to read (Stein & Beed, Citation2004) and enjoyment of reading (Cameron et al., Citation2012). Grace, Lucita, and Ximena vocalized to me an improvement in their enjoyment of reading, whereas Tomás and Will demonstrated to both Mrs. Ian and me an improvement in their enjoyment of reading.

Literature circles provide a context for students to model their thought processes for the other group members (V. Smith, Citation2012). As mentioned previously, a student’s observation of others can influence his or her self-efficacy (Schunk & Zimmerman, Citation2007). Linking the two together, previous research conducted by Hall (Citation2012) found that text-based discussion groups can benefit struggling readers, as these readers have the opportunity to observe their peers utilizing various reading strategies. This study expands on Hall’s findings by suggesting that literature circles, which revolve around text-based discussion, can potentially improve the reader self-efficacies of reluctant and struggling readers.

Limitations

There are two primary limitations to this study. It is unknown whether reluctant and/or struggling readers were present in the pool of other study participants. However, I took steps to mitigate this issue by conferring with Mrs. Ian to identify potential cases for further study. Moreover, Mrs. Ian did not identify any of the other study participants—who demonstrated initially higher reader self-efficacies—as either reluctant or struggling readers. Prior research by Valdez (Citation2013) supports the validity of teachers’ judgements of their students’ literacy skills. Consequently, I trusted Mrs. Ian’s professional judgment, especially because she had taught some of the cases during the previous school year(s).

As an embedded multiple case study, this study was primarily qualitative in nature; therefore, I cannot speak to the generalizability of the results and findings. Moreover, as this was a case study, the cases and other study participants were bound to the unique context of Mrs. Ian’s Grade 4–6 classroom. Therefore, the results and findings that emerged from this study may not translate to other contexts. The quantitative data collected for this study served two purposes: (a) to help identify potential cases for further study and (b) to supplement the qualitative data. Thus, the sample size (i.e., n = 7 for cases, n = 10 for other study participants) is too small to warrant deeper analysis. Furthermore, I used criterion-based purposeful sampling to identify the seven cases. All students took part in literature circles; therefore, there was neither a treatment nor a control group.

Future research

Future research into literature circles or text-based discussion groups and reader self-efficacy is needed, particularly regarding the reader self-efficacies of students who tend to be more successful in more traditional literacy instructional contexts than reluctant or struggling readers. I suggest that quasi-experimental studies be conducted on the potential influence of literature circles or text-based discussion groups on reader self-efficacy. Conducting such quasi-experimental research in separate classrooms would allow for the creation of both a control group (i.e., students who do not participate in literature circles) and a treatment group (i.e., students who do participate in literature circles). Nevertheless, this study suggests that literature circles may influence the reader self-efficacies of reluctant and struggling readers.

Conclusion

The results and findings of this study suggest that participating in a literature circle can potentially strengthen the reader self-efficacies of reluctant and struggling readers. This study is significant because similar results were not found for the other study participants with initially higher reader self-efficacies. Thus, this study provides support for the utilization of literature circles—a balanced literacy instructional strategy that is engaging, student centered, and holistic—to potentially boost the reader self-efficacies of reluctant and struggling readers in Grades 4–6. As mentioned previously, Grade 4–6 students often fail to calibrate their self-efficacies and reading abilities (Corkett et al., Citation2011; Linnenbrink & Pintrich, Citation2003; Pajares, Citation2003; J. K. Smith et al., Citation2012). I argue that literature circles—an engaging, more student-centered, and holistic approach to reading instruction—merits inclusion in Grade 4–6 classrooms for its potential to strengthen the reader self-efficacies of reluctant and struggling readers.

References

Reprints and Corporate Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

To request a reprint or corporate permissions for this article, please click on the relevant link below:

Academic Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

Obtain permissions instantly via Rightslink by clicking on the button below:

If you are unable to obtain permissions via Rightslink, please complete and submit this Permissions form. For more information, please visit our Permissions help page.