ABSTRACT
The phenomenon of multi-used stock photography in the news contradicts the photojournalism professional values of truthful and emotional depictions. This reality echoes other false images increasingly appearing in the media, including deepfakes and artificial intelligence. In the present study, a two (stock and staff photo) by two (positive and negative valence) quasi-experiment is conducted. The dependent variables include: 1) credibility; 2) self-reported arousal level; 3) emotional valence perceptions; 4) fixation duration; and 5) fixation count. Participants viewed staff photos as more credible in both positive and negative valences. Negative photos were perceived as slightly less credible. Stock and staff photos were similarly arousing, received similar valence perceptions, and had similar gaze fixations and durations. Overall, participants perceived the genuine emotion portrayed truthfully in photojournalism as opposed to the faux emotion acted out in stock photos.
The phenomenon of untruthful photography appearing in journalistic contexts flies in the face of the photojournalistic ethical imperative to truthfully depict and document the emotion inherent in the human experience. Heavily manipulated images, a concern that rose at the turn of the century, are joined with AI-generated images, deepfakes, and stock image banks, creating a slew of potentially deceitful imagery in the media. Creative stock photos, the subject of this study, are not truthful and do not depict genuine emotion. While editorial stock image banks do exist, the subject of the present study are those with staged scenes, created with the intent of multiple uses, and customizable to various needs. These stock photos are often used as a means of acquiring low-cost visuals (Adobe, Citation2023). Found historically primarily within the persuasive media realm, these stock photos also sometimes appear in the news (Frosh, Citation2001) and social media sites. Photographs taken by staffed photojournalists, on the other hand, are shot with a professed commitment to ethical values (Elliott, Citation2003; Fahmy et al., Citation2014). What separates a journalist from other content creators, Singer (Citation2007) say, are “ethics or norms, as well as the principles that underlie those norms” (p. 47).
A most esteemed journalistic ethical principle is truth (Kovach & Rosenstiel, Citation2021). Untruthful visual news can be harmful to viewers who rely on it to make informed decisions; it also can ultimately damage the credibility of the news industry (Elliott, Citation2011; Silva & Eldridge II, Citation2020; Self, Citation2014). Credibility encompasses other variables related to truth, including accuracy, completeness, objectivity, and trust.
Depicting the emotion involved in the human experience is a second hallmark norm of photojournalism (Lester, Citation2018; Newton, Citation2013; Rössler et al., Citation2011). Emotion is characterized by valence (negative or positive tone) and arousal (high intensity or low intensity; Bradley & Lang, Citation1994). Photos with a negative valence are the most common and most rewarded in photojournalism (Godulla et al., Citation2021; Singletary & Lamb, Citation1984; Shoemaker & Reese, Citation2013). Negative photos can increase outcomes such as attention, memory, assessments, and news judgment (Calvo & Lang, Citation2004; Emmett, Citation2011; Knobloch et al., Citation2003; Martin-Kratzer, Citation2005; Rössler et al., Citation2011; Smith-Rodden & Ash, Citation2012). For example, a March 2022 photo of rescue workers evacuating a pregnant woman from a Ukrainian maternity ward after a Russian attack impacted how Americans visualize and perceive the war (Bracaglia, Citation2022).
Given these principles, using stock photos in a news context has caused concern among ethicists and journalists alike. When the Poynter Institute suggested the use of free stock photos by journalists to use in articles in 2018, members of the National Press Photographer’s Association, Associated Press Photo Managers, and The Kalish Visual Editing Workshop expressed “profound disappointment” (Poynter Staff, para 1). They cited the potential for accidentally using unlicensed images and the risk of obtaining an image that has been manipulated. They further pointed out that the interaction between an image and a story may lead to false conclusions. Finally, they argued that audiences recognize and appreciate the quality of professional photos. Much of the debate about using stock photos revolves around the question of whether audiences can detect stock images as such. Sonderman (Citation2013) notes, for example, that unless stock photos are blatantly obvious, “at best” the reader is uncertain about whether a photo is stock (para 6; see also Staff, Citation2018). Beyond occasional anecdotes (e.g. O’Rourke, Citation2022; Putterman, Citation2021), there is not currently a solid understanding of whether and in which what ways audiences are being deceived by stock photos, a potential harm to viewers and photographed alike (Elliott, Citation2011). In March 2022, users incorrectly scrutinized the authenticity of a photo of Joe Biden on Twitter (now X) due to the green foliage in the background (O’Rourke, Citation2022). But viewers were correct in their detection of the falsehood of an image published by the New York Times when one user posted “SHAMEFUL – The New York Times publish a 2015 stock photo of a young girl claiming Israeli forces killed her during last week’s war with Hamas.” In this case, the photo was from 2015 and posted incorrectly with a similar story in 2020 (Putterman, Citation2021), leaving the impression that, as one poster noted, the girl to seem to be “killed twice.”
Truth and emotion are distinctly photojournalistic axioms, but blatantly false photos can and do exist in news contexts. While scholars and news professionals recognize the ethical concerns, little is known about audience detection of and deception by false images appearing in a news context. Learning whether and in what ways audiences can perceive the difference between an untrue stock photo and a truthful staff-created photo has implications for the credibility of professional photojournalism and ramifications for an industry with both an historical and contemporary devaluation of photojournalists (Klein-Avraham & Reich, Citation2016; Zelizer, Citation1995). The implications of this study can be extended into areas such as ChatGPT and other artificial-intelligence created image, as well. Learning about the way audiences can or cannot detect subtle cues indicating visual falsehoods may help justify the use of professionally-shot news staff photographs. Further, learning about the ways audiences are being deceived or not will help inform news organizations’ future ethical decision-making around the use of stock photos.
The purpose of this study is to examine the effects of emotional valence and source upon viewer perceptions of a stock and staff photo in a news context. A two (source; stock versus photojournalist) by two (negative versus positive valence) quasi-experiment is undertaken. Replicating and augmenting previous studies, viewer perceptions are measured with five dependent variables: 1) credibility perceptions using the news-photo credibility scale; 2) self-reported arousal level, 3) emotional valence perceptions, 4) fixation duration, and 5) fixation count.
Evolving state of news visuals
The photojournalist layoffs of the last couple decades caused by evolving business models and a long history of photojournalists viewed as second-class newsroom citizens has left visual vacancies in the news (Brennen, Citation1998b; Gade, Citation2011; Klein-Avraham & Reich, Citation2016; Lydersen, Citation2013; Zelizer, Citation1995). As online sources of visuals have become hugely abundant and digital technologies afford nearly everyone to take a technically-sound photograph, photojournalists are viewed as a relatively disposable newsroom asset (Brennen & Brennen, Citation2015; Klein-Avraham & Reich, Citation2016; Lowrey, Citation2002). Some have said that it seems almost any type of visual will suffice, including citizen-shot photos, file photos, or generic stock photos (Klein-Avraham & Reich, Citation2016; Sonderman, Citation2013). Sometimes news sites “just need a photo, any photo, to color an otherwise gray slate of text” (Sonderman, Citation2013).
Stock photography
Photographers of non-editorial stock images create photos that anticipate the future needs of various media outlets with the ultimate goal toward breadth of applications. Getty Images and others offer both stock and journalistic imagery, which it refers to as either “creative” or “editorial” (Gaston, Citationn.d.). The “creative” style of stock photos is the subject of the present research. Since its 1974 inception, the stock photo industry has grown greatly (Frosh, Citation2001). In 1995, Getty photos created the first online photo bank, increasing availability (Machin, Citation2004). The Global Stock Photos Market Report noted $2 billion in revenue in 2020 with a projected 7% annual growth through 2026 (Expert Market Research, 2021). The nature of the industry has led to the stereotypical generic stock photo: a “glossy, formulaic, multi-purpose representation” (Frosh, Citation2001, p. 30). “Good” stock photos are said to have vivid colors, perfect white balance, a sharp subject, typically with a blurred background, low ISO for crisp focus, and a horizontal orientation (Wallon-Hárs, Citation2023). As for the content, stock photos have been described as having “a weird, posey cheesiness often unified by hilariously bad photo editing” (Baliūnaitė, Citation2022). As Baliūnaitė (Citation2022) claims, “When you see a stock photo, you know it’s a stock photo” (para 1, italics in original). And yet, stock photos have been called “the visual backbone” not only of advertising, branding, and publishing, but also “journalism” (Aiello, Citation2016, n.p; Frosh, Citation2001). In more recent years, stock photographers have moved toward a more authentic-appearing aesthetic (Aiello, Citation2016). The rise of smartphones has driven a trend toward less technically perfect and sterile shots (“7 photographers,” 2018). These conceptual and visual changes can be ethically worrisome to those in the journalism industry, with lines blurring between posed and authentic.
Photojournalism ethics and norms
While content is abundantly available online, what distinguishes journalism from other activities related to gathering, editing, and disseminating information to the public is its ethos (Singer, Citation2007). Photojournalists working in a newsroom traditionally profess to abide by ethical standards and are guided by deep-seated principles (Fahmy et al., Citation2014; Silva & Eldridge II, Citation2020). News photos have been credited with having the “extraordinary power to move readers’ consciences and prompt action (Santos & Eldridge II, Citation2020, p. 2), though some scholars challenge the assumed impact of photos (e.g., Perlmutter, Citation1998). Elliott (Citation2003) advise thatbecause visuals have the emotional power to educate, entertain, and persuade, there is a responsibility put on each photo created for the public (p. 24).
Truth, harm, and credibility in photojournalism
Truth and harm
Truth is often considered journalism’s most important ethical principle, and falsehoods have the potential to cause harm (Elliott, Citation2011; Kovach & Rosenstiel, Citation2021; Lester, Citation2018; Silva & Eldridge II, Citation2020, p. 31). Western ideals of the press stem from a mission to uphold democracy through freedom of speech and press, disseminating to the public information required for them to make decisions (Merrill, Citation1974; Silva and Eldridge II, Citation2020). Visual truth, “authentic knowledge derived from seeing,” echos this ethos in photojournalism (Newton, Citation2013, pg. 8). Newton asserts “The heart of photojournalism is reporting the human experience accurately, honestly, and with an overriding sense of social responsibility” (p. x). Deni Elliot recognizes the potential for false visual information to cause harm, regardless of whether the harm is intended. For example, the reputation of the person or people in the photo can be stereotyped or harmed in other ways; viewers are harmed by making decisions based on misinformation and are deprived of “the opportunity to grapple with truthful information and come to decisions” (pg. 13). If the truth is eventually revealed, viewers are harmed again because they are likely to become less trusting of news photos. Ultimately, the media that distributed the deceptive picture are harmed by a loss of credibility (Elliott, Citation2011).
The National Press Photographer Association’s (2022) code of ethics is clear regarding its stance on truth. The first rule admonishes photojournalists to be “accurate and comprehensive in the representation of subjects.” The second rule commands they resist staged photo opportunities. Rule three asks photojournalists to be complete and provide context. Photojournalists are told to avoid presenting their own biases in the fourth rule. Rule five encourages them to not alter or influence events. Finally, rule six states, “editing should maintain the integrity of the photographic images content and context” (para 11). In the preamble, it is stated: “Our primary goal is the faithful and comprehensive depiction of the subject at hand. As visual journalists, we have the responsibility to document society and to preserve its history through photos” (para 2). As Julianne Newton predicted in 2001, “Good visual reportage may very well be the only credible source of reasonably true photos in decades to come” (pg. x).
Credibility
Credibility refers to whether audiences view news as truthful, and thus whether their trust has been earned (Metzger et al., Citation2003; Self, Citation2014; Sundar & Nass, Citation2001). The concept relies upon viewer perceptions (Self, Citation2014; Tseng & Fogg, Citation1999); for example, how viewers perceive the trustworthiness of a source, medium, or message (e.g., Gaziano & McGrath, Citation1986; Kiousis, Citation2001; Metzger et al., Citation2003). Since its mid-century application to journalism, scholars have broken down the broad construct into multiple concepts and variables. In 1951, Hovland and Weiss named two basic components of credibility: trustworthiness and expertise. In the following decades, researchers further subdivided credibility into more specific variables, including accuracy, fairness, completeness, reliability, motivation for money, respect of privacy, community well-being, objectivity, competence, and trustworthiness (e.g., Gaziano & McGrath, Citation1986; Kiousis, Citation2001; e.g.; Metzger et al., Citation2003; Meyer, Citation1988 Rimmer & Weaver, Citation1987).
Credibility studies pertaining to visuals are less prominent. The nuances of photo incompleteness, subjectivity, and alteration have been deliberated since long before digital editing was mainstream. Until recently, visual credibility research has focused almost exclusively on issues of alterations and editing (e.g., Greer & Gosen, Citation2002; Schwartz, Citation2020; Wheeler & Gleason Citation1995). Attention to visual source credibility has become more relevant. For example, Gayle (Citation2018) examined the perceived credibility of professional photojournalism to user-generated content (UGC), finding that UGC is perceived as less credible when the platform and photographer are identified. In 2021, Mortensen, McDermott and Ejaz looked at the credibility perceptions of stock photos versus professional photojournalist photos, finding that participants self-reported stock photos as significantly less credible. The researchers created and published a reliable scale that measures photo credibility in journalistic contexts. This scale is used in the present study.
Emotion
Photojournalists-in-training have been coached to capture the emotion involved in the human experience since virtually the development of the profession (Costa, Citation1950). Emotion is defined in psychology by the dimensions of valence and arousal (Barrett et al., Citation2007; Lang & Bradley, Citation2010). Textbooks consistently urge students to both capture emotion and thus to evoke emotion among viewers (e.g. Arthur, Citation2016; Horton, Citation1989; Kobré, Citation2011; Skinner, Citation2010). Intimate depictions of emotion is a key consideration used to judge contests, with more emotional photos winning more awards (Lough, Citation2021; Singletary & Lamb, Citation1984). Professional photographs, overall, show more emotion than nonprofessional photography (Greenwood & Thomas, Citation2015; Mortensen, et al., Citation2023). Editorial departments, too, tend to choose photos that show greater emotion (Rössler et al., Citation2011). Emotional photography is necessarily intertwined with notions of truth; the dramatization of emotion in stock photos are explicit falsehoods, infringing on photojournalism ethos.
Negative emotion
The emotions that photojournalists depict tend toward the negative. In 1984, Singletary and Lamb looked at award-winning photos from 1978 and 1981, finding that 81% of winning photos were of accidents, disaster, crime, or violence. Only three of the 222 photos were clearly positive. Similar results were found in a content analysis of all photos from 1960 to 2020 (Godulla et al., Citation2021). So prominent are negative news photos that, in one study, 2021 was the first and only time a winning photo was positive in tone (see also Lough, Citation2021). This negative slant reflects the news values of conflict, human interest, and the unusual (Caple & Bednarek, Citation2016; Conley & Lamble, Citation2006; Galtung & Ruge, Citation1965). These values are found in photojournalism, as well (Bednarek & Caple, Citation2012; Craig, Citation1994; Singletary & Lamb, Citation1984).
Effects of emotional photos
Eye-tracking studies measure gaze patterns, fixation count, and fixation duration and have provided evidence of effects of emotional photos. Valence denotes a positive or negative reaction (Lang, Greenwald, Bradley, & Hamm, Citation1993), and arousal refers to the level of stimulation (Berger & Milkman, Citation2013). While gaze patterns and duration do not equate attention, tracking eye movements has been used to study visual attention (Isaacowitz et al., Citation2006). Studies have found that both pleasant and unpleasant pictures are more likely to be looked at – and for longer – than neutral pictures (Calvo & Lang, Citation2004); emotion can drive attention (e.g., Eastwood et al., Citation2001; Fenske & Eastwood, Citation2003; Fox et al., Citation2001); and focusing on negative photos can result in a negative response, while focusing on positive photos is more likely to lead to a positive response. Attention can also drive and modulate subsequent affective emotional responses (Fenske et al., Citation2004; Raymond et al., Citation2003).
Studies of press photography have used different, but related variables; for example, attention, recall, interest, beliefs and intention. Reflecting high arousal and negative valence, articles accompanied with threatening photos have been shown to foster greater attention (Knobloch et al., Citation2003); those emphasizing damage and violence have been associated with increased attention, higher photo recall, better evaluations, and an increased interest (Rössler et al., Citation2011); negative and cataclysmic photos received higher emotional ratings than positive photos (Emmett, Citation2011); more intense photos have been associated with greater influence on emotional assessments and news judgment, particularly if they triggered a negative emotion (Martin-Kratzer, Citation2005). In a study by Gelpi and Gartner (Citation2011), exposure to the negative photos about the Afghan war dramatically increased subjects’ beliefs about losing the war in Afghanistan, but the positive and neutral photos had no effect. Similarly, Smith-Rodden and Ash (Citation2017) found that more emotional photos tended toward greater influence on support for African Famine Aid and military action in Afghanistan.
Research questions, and hypotheses
The literature shows that truth and emotion are photojournalistic axioms with ethical implications, but very little is known about perceptions of photos that are traditionally neither truthful nor emotional: multi-purpose stock photos. Related studies have suggested that photo source can affect audience perceptions, that audiences often react more strongly to negatively-toned photos, and that emotional photos are sometimes associated with emotional reactions. Learning whether viewers perceive the difference between a stock and staff photo will begin to help shed light on the degree to which viewers might be deceived, ultimately harming industry credibility. In this study, we respond to a call for increased empirical research exploring perceptions of news photographs (Müller et al., Citation2012; Smith-Rodden & Ash, Citation2012). Specifically, we look at participants’ perceptions in a 2 (source; stock versus photojournalist) x 2 (negative versus positive valence) quasi-experiment, measuring: 1) self-reported arousal level, 2) self-reported valence perceptions 3) credibility perceptions, 4) fixation duration, and 5) fixation count.
Replicating and augmenting Mortensen and colleagues (2021), we hypothesize:
H1:
Respondents will perceive staff photos as more credible than stock photos a) overall, b) with positive photos and c) with negative photos.
Adding to self-reports of credibility perception and moving toward psychological, quantitative measures we ask:
RQ1:
Is the source of the photo associated with different a) self-reported arousal-levels, b) valence perceptions c) fixation duration, and d) fixation counts?
And adding to literature most often found within the psychology realm and primarily assumed by the news industry, we ask:
RQ2:
Is the valence of photos associated with greater a) credibility perceptions, b) self-reported arousal levels, c) valence perceptions, d) fixation duration, and e) fixation counts?
Method
Quasi-experimental design and procedure
A two (valence: positive vs. negative) x two (photo source: stock vs. professional photojournalist) mixed-factor design quasi-experiment was conducted. The quasi-experiment was conducted in a laboratory setting equipped for eye-tracking research. What makes the design a quasi-experiment is the nature of photos themselves. We did not isolate a single variable, such as a cutline. Rather, we were interested in whether participants could perceive the nuanced differences between the two types of photos. Other variables were necessary and unavoidable. Participants were invited and informed about the study via SONA, an internal recruitment system at the research institution. Participants were sent a confirmation e-mail that included an informed consent letter. Participants were asked to sit in a separate room behind a 22-inch monitor at a distance of 60 cm. A Tobii X3–120 eye-tracker was attached. The researcher sat at a separate station outside of the room. Eye-tracking data were collected using a sample gaze rate of 120 Hz per second, and gaze samples were recorded for eye fixations of about every 8 milliseconds. Once calibration was completed, participants were instructed to look at either a positive or a negative stimuli in random order for 30 seconds each that appeared in the form of an editorial regarding opioid addiction (See ). Participants were randomly assigned to view the two different valenced photos taken by either stock photographers or professional photojournalists. They did not know which photo source condition they were given, as determining whether the photo itself affected perceptions was a purpose of the study. Participants were then asked to complete a questionnaire about their emotional responses toward each photo and the perceived photo credibility, as well as demographic information. Participants were thanked and compensated with a $10 gift card.
Participants
A total of 97 undergraduate student participants were recruited at a large public university. Four were removed from data analyses for completing the wrong survey for their assigned condition. Two more were removed for having not recorded any eye tracking data. Nine other participants were also removed because they did not meet the 75% threshold for data quality. 41 participants remained for each condition (N = 82).
Among the participants, 12.2% were male (N = 10), 84.1% were female (N = 69), and 3.7% identified as other (N = 3). In terms of ethnicity, 43.9% of participants were White (N = 36), 50% reported as Black or African American (N = 41), 1.2% were Asian (N = 1), and 4.9% identified as Other Ethnicity (N = 4). The age of participants ranged from 18–26, with a mean of 21. One participant was removed from this statistical calculation for an incomplete response.
Stimuli development
The team began by finding photojournalistic photos concerning the broad theme of drug abuse, addiction and recovery. This theme offered a variety of photos with varying degrees of emotional salience and intensity and news photos about the topic are often accompanied by stock photos. Through search engines and searches of award-winning photojournalism projects, a professional photojournalist on the research team found 25 potential photos taken by 12 photojournalists. Award-winning photos were chosen because of their adherence to photojournalistic standards and vetting by the profession. The majority of these photographs were taken in the past decade. The researchers discussed the salience of the photos, and narrowed the collection down to ten promisingly evocative stock stimuli.
Next, a search of the tens of millions of stock photos available through the iStock website began. Finding an appropriately balanced comparison between a stock photo and a staff photo was challenging; if the photos are too perfect a conceptual and aesthetic match, there is nothing to compare. On the other hand, if too many variables differ, one cannot pinpoint which variable led to differing results. Hours were spent finding stock photos that approximated the storytelling moment, and further matched the number of people in the frame, as well as their gender and ethnicity. Though the photos were not exact compositional matches in all cases, they all were taken in the same compositional spirit: for example, with a wide-angle lens, or from a slightly higher angle.
A pretest (N = 213) was then conducted on Amazon Mechanical Turk. Each participant was asked to evaluate their emotional responses (in terms of valence and arousal) to three random photos from the pool of 20 photos (10 stock and 10 photojournalistic). As this study intended to manipulate the positive and negative valence triggered by the stock and photojournalistic photos, the team selected one pair of positive and one pair of negative photos, while both photos elicited a similar level of arousal. Based on the one-way ANOVA test results, the team selected one positive stock photo (M = 6.30, M = 4.06) and one positive photojournalistic photo (M = 5.64, M = 4.58), one negative stock photo (M = 3.32, M = 4.48) and one negative photojournalistic photo (M = 3.09, M = 4.79). Both pairs (i.e., positive/negative stock photos and positive/negative photojournalistic photos) were significantly different in the valence scores but not different in the arousal scores.
Lastly, based on the selected four photos, the team created four different stimuli to be used in the main study. Each photo, whether stock or staff, was placed in the same generic local news presentation, with a headline that said “Local News” and a generic subheadline explaining the main action of the photograph.
Dependent variables
Photo credibility
The current study measures credibility by adopting Mortensen and colelagues’ (Citation2023) credibility scale for photos appearing in the news. This scale uses nine dimensions (18 items) to measure the perceived credibility of photos: Authority, accuracy, coverage, completeness, currency, journalism professionalism, objectivity, photojournalism professionalism, and trust. Each dimension was measured using two 5-point Likert-type items ranging from Strongly disagree to Strongly agree. See for details.
Table 1. Photo credibility in journalistic contexts (Mortensen et al., Citation2023)
Arousal
Arousal was measured using three 9-point items on a bipolar scale: relaxed/stimulated, calm/excited, and unaroused/aroused (Gorn et al., Citation2001).
Valence perceptions
Valence was measured using four 9-point items on a bipolar scale: sad/happy, depressed/joyful, displeased/pleased, and distressed/delighted (Gorn et al., Citation2001).
Fixation count
Past eye-tracking studies have focused on fixation count as a dependent variable (Simmonds et al., Citation2023). The current study measures fixation count as the number of times eye movement was spent fixated on a particular area of interest.
Fixation duration
Fixation duration is a reputable measure in eye-tracking research (Marquart et al., Citation2023). The current study measured fixation duration as the average time spent dwelling on a particular area of interest.
Results
Hypotheses 1 proposed that respondents would perceive staff images as more credible than stock images a) overall, b) with positive images and c) with negative images. All three parts of the hypothesis were supported. The two-way Univariate Analysis of Variance (ANOVA) showed that a) staff photos (M = 3.20, SD = .56) received significantly greater credibility perceptions than stock photos (M = 2.92, SD = .63); (F(1,160) = [11.09], p < .001, p2 = .07). This was true for negative and positive photos, with respective mean differences of .27 and .29. Staff photos are perceived as more credible than stock photos on almost all scale items, with two exceptions. To the Professionalism item, which includes the statements, “A skilled photographer took this photo” and “Anybody could take a photo of this quality” (stated inversely), those who viewed stock photos were significantly more in agreement with the professionalism concept, with significant mean differences of .28 (negative) and 1.07 (positive) photos. Staff images are also perceived as being significantly less “current” than stock images on both negative (mean difference = .18) and positive valence conditions (mean difference = .16).
RQ1 asked whether the source of the image was associated with different a) different self-reported arousal-levels, b) different valence perceptions c) gaze fixation and d) gaze duration, and whether there were any interaction effects for each variable. The answer is mixed. Results showed no significant main effects, but some significant interaction effects.
a) There was no significant main difference in self-reported arousal level between those who viewed photos taken by stock photographers and those who viewed the staff photo; (F(1,160) = .41, p = .53, p2 = .003). However, there were significant interaction effects. Viewers reported higher arousal when viewing negative stock images (M = 5.61, SD = 1.31) than the negative staff images (M = 4.89, SD = 1.46, p = .041), [MT1] (F(1,160) = 5.160, p = .024, p2 = .031), and negative stock photos (M = 5.61, SD = 1.31) were also viewed as more arousing than the positive stock photos. (M = 4.28, SD = 1.92, p < .001)
b) There were no main or interaction significant differences in valence perceptions between those who viewed stock photos and staff photos.
c) Regarding whether there was a significant association between source and gaze fixation, the answer is no. However, interaction effects between emotion and source on fixation count was significant (F(1,160) = 3.959, p = .048, p2 = .023). Participants who viewed stock images had greater fixation counts on negative images (M = 46.55, SD = 18.57) than positive images. (M = 35.64, SD = 15.22, p = .005)
d) There was no significant main effect of source on fixation duration. There was a statistically significant interaction effect between emotion and source on fixation duration (F(1,160) = 4.139, p = .043, p2 = .024). Participants who viewed stock images spent a longer time viewing negative images (M = 10658.93, SD = 4328.46) than positive images (M = 7825.68, SD = 3874.25, p = .007). Additionally, participants who viewed positive photos spent a longer time looking at the staff photo (M = 9868.75, SD = 5167.68) than stock photo. (M = 7825.68, SD = 3847.25, p = .047)
Research question two asked whether the valence of images is associated with greater a) credibility, b) self-reported arousal levels, c) valence perception, d) fixation counts, and e) fixation duration. Results of the two-way ANOVA tests show that:
a) Positive images (M = 3.16, SD = 0.51) were associated with significantly greater credibility perceptions than negative photos (M = 2.96, SD = 0.58); F (1,160) = 6.17, p = .014, p2 = .037).
b) Those who viewed a positive image (M = 7.31, SD = 1.26) reported much more positive emotional responses compared to those who viewed a negative image (M = 2.52, SD = 1.20, F (1,160) = 616.10, p < .001, p2 = .794).
c) Those who viewed a negative image reported significantly higher arousal (M = 5.25, SD = 1.42) than those who viewed a positive image (M = 4.48, SD = 1.78); (F(1,160) = 9.541, p = .002, p2 = .056),
d) Negative photos (M = 43.28, SD = 18.91) received more fixation counts than positive images (M = 37.81, SD = 16.23); (F(1,160) = 4.375, p = .038, p2 = .025),
e) There was no significant difference between image valence and fixation duration.
Discussion
This study sought to examine perceptions of staff and stock photos with different valences. The results of the study both confirm and contradict the literature. Overall, participants viewed staff photos as generally more credible, and this applied to both positive and negative valences. In both stock photos and staff photos, negative photos were perceived as slightly less credible. Stock and staff photos were similarly arousing, received similar valence perceptions, and were associated with similar gaze fixations and durations, overall. Interaction effects in terms of source were apparent, though, with stock photos being associated with greater effects in most instances. In terms of valence differences, negative photos were much more arousing and received greater fixation counts. Positive images, opposed to negative images, received much more positive emotional responses.
That participants’ perceptions of the credibility of staff photos were greater than stock photos was a finding consistent with Mortensen and colleagues (Citation2023) who also used the same scale to test credibility perceptions of stock and staff photos. This finding bolsters the argument made by members of professional photography associations NPPA, APPM, and The Kalish that audiences can detect between professional and other kinds of photos, spending more time with professional ones. As they say, “It’s not good enough to simply have a photo with your story. The content of images and captions matter” (Staff, Citation2018, para. 9). The mean difference of .28% points was modest but significant. Overall, participants perceived the genuine emotion portrayed truthfully in photojournalism as opposed to the faux emotion acted out in stock photos. The pattern of credibility perceptions was the same with both valence conditions. This replication adds validity to the scale and reinforces the notion that audiences perceive greater overall credibility of staff photos. Knowing that audiences can detect some differences between staff and stock photos supports the argument that quality photojournalism matters. The one scale item in which the stock photo condition was rated as more credible in the Mortensen and colleagues (Citation2023) study – professionalism–was also rated more credible in the stock condition in the present study. Viewers’ perceptions that stock photos are more “professional” in this regard might reflect the squeaky-clean, technically sleek nature of traditional stock photos (Frosh, Citation2001; Wallon-Hárs, Citation2023). Stock photos are in fact taken by skilled photographers who have the time to stage the photo and get the lighting and composition perfect in a way that on-staff photojournalists might not, thus lending to what viewers may perceive as a “professional” appearance.
Importantly, the one statement with no significant difference concerned whether the photos were relevant and specific to the story. While the fact that only one statement had no significance may at face value seem to imply that there is very little problem with using stock photos, this particular statement is telling. The insignificance suggests that while audiences seem to note differences between the two types of photos, they may not understand that stock photos were not taken specifically for the story at hand. This contradicts what is known about stock photos – they are created to accompany many contexts (Aiello, Citation2016). This suggests that viewers are being deceived in perceiving some stock photos in the news as taken specifically for the news story. The Poynter Institute (Staff, Citation2018) notes that not fully considering the interaction between an image and the story can lead to false impressions about a subject, a point that should help guide newsrooms’ policies on the use of stock photos and inform education in photojournalism. In the same way the profession and higher education warn against staging and altering photos and when minor alterations might be deemed acceptable, there should be serious discussion as to when, if, and in what manner stock photos should ever be used in a news context.
The findings regarding valence are somewhat consistent with literature and with the journalism industry’s previous values and patterns, but more mixed. The journalism industry’s pattern toward the negative was suggested to be shared by audiences in the present study (e.g., Godulla et al., Citation2021; Lough, Citation2021; Singletary & Lamb, Citation1984). Negative photos generated much higher arousal, with a large mean difference of .77. This was the strongest difference in the study. Studies have consistently shown that negative photos are rewarded in the news industry (e.g., Lough, Citation2021), and this study shows there is a payoff for doing so. However, negative photos did not drive greater attention as the literature would suggest (Knobloch et al., Citation2003; Rössler et al., Citation2011), except in the case of the stock photos, wherein they had greater fixation counts and duration. As these stock photos were dramatized, it seems they may be more attention-grabbing than the staff photos. The ability of stock photos to gain attention does not compensate for their ethical breaches. Interestingly, positive photos – regardless of source – were perceived as more credible than negative photos. One explanation is also a limitation of the study: given the quasi-experimental photographic study design, the stimulus materials were not identical with one isolated different variable. The positive photos in this study were in color, while the negative photos were black and white, for example. Another explanation is that the negative photos were viewed as sensational, thus less credible. While audiences might be drawn to the negative photos, the findings suggest that can detect at least some of their untruthful nature. In the case of the negative photos, both portray a woman injecting heroin, but the stock photo shows slightly more face, the needle is slightly more prominent, and it should be addressed that the clothing of the woman on the stock condition is more revealing. While it might be interpreted that the differences in the stimulus materials caused the results, it should be noted that the well-thought-out composition is afforded through the staged photos in the stock condition, but not the staff condition.
These results are difficult to interpret, but the researchers must concede that some of the perhaps unexpected results are likely due to the stimulus materials. Every step was taken to find comparable photos, but the nature of this kind of visual communication research is that it is impossible to isolate a single variable while using real-world photos. If the photos were identical except for a cutline, it would be impossible to gauge whether viewers actually perceive the different sources. Given this complication, the findings of this study are not generalizable but rather one piece of evidence in the pursuit of audience perceptions of stock or otherwise untruthful imagery. We urge more studies to replicate the present one and help control for the varying nature of photos, however slight. Another limitation of the study worth mentioning is the sample; only undergraduates at a single university participated in the study. This was a necessary limitation given the in-person nature of the lab setting.
The insignificant results are telling, as well. Stock and staff photos received similar valence perceptions, overall, which is one measure of emotion. In other words, they did not foster stronger negative or positive perceptions, overall. The two sources also fostered similar main effects for fixation duration and fixation, suggesting they draw similar levels of attention, though there are nuanced interaction effects with valence, as discussed above. These similarities, again, show a level of deception among viewers who see stock photos in the news.
This study begins to reveal some potential ethical implications of generic stock photos in the news. The findings are mostly positive for professional photojournalists and their viewers in that they hint at the importance of photojournalism. At the broadest level, they suggest viewers recognize the genuine, emotional nature of professional photojournalism when compared with stock photos. Still, more nuanced findings show that in some ways, viewers are being deceived and perceiving false images as truthful. Specifically, audiences do not necessarily understand that the photograph is not taken specifically for the story at hand, even though they can detect other visual differences between stock and staff photos. This deception has the potential to harm viewers trying to make sense of a story through the combination of words and visuals, the shaky credibility of the news industry, and – as is was the case with the New York Times anecdote – the subjects of photos. The initial results should give pause to an industry quick to dispose of photojournalists first when budgets are tight. The appropriateness of using stock photos in the news merits serious contemplation amongst professionals and educators. Future research into false imagery in the news could extend the present research into the arenas of artificial intelligence and deepfakes.
Disclosure statement
No potential conflict of interest was reported by the author(s).
Additional information
Funding
References
- Adobe. (2023). Stock photography - what it is & how to use it. https://www.adobe.com/creativecloud/photography/discover/stock-photography.html(open in a new window)
- Aiello, G. (2016, April 28). Taking stock. Ethnography Matters. http://ethnographymatters.net/blog/2016/04/28/taking-stock(open in a new window)
- Arthur, R. (2016). Phone photography for everybody: iPhone photojournalism techniques. Mass.
- Baliūnaitė, I. (2022, August 31). People are sharing hilariously bad stock photos of their jobs, and you’ll laugh out loud when you find yours. Bored Panda. Retrieved September 14, 2022, from. https://www.boredpanda.com/funny-bad-stock-photos-of-jobs-badstockphotosofmyjob/(open in a new window)
- Barrett, L. F., Mesquita, B., Ochsner, K. N., & Gross, J. J. (2007). The experience of emotion. Annual Review of Psychology, 58, 373–403. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.58.110405.085709
- Bednarek, M., & Caple, H. (2012). ‘Value added’: Language, image and news values. Discourse, Context, Media, 1(2–3), 103–113. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dcm.2012.05.006
- Berger, J., & Milkman, K. L. (2013). Emotion and virality: What makes online content go viral?. NIM Marketing Intelligence Review, 5(1), 18–23.
- Bracaglia, D. (2022, March 16). This Photo Could Define the War in Ukraine for generations. Popular Photography. Retrieved September 14, 2022, from https://www.popphoto.com/news/ukriane-war-maternity-hospital-tragedy/(open in a new window)
- Bradley, M. M., & Lang, P. J. (1994). Measuring emotion: The self-assessment manikin and the semantic differential. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 25(1), 49–59. https://doi.org/10.1016/0005-7916(94)90063-9
- Brennen, B. (1998b). Strategic competition and the value of photographers’ work: Photojournalism in. American Journalism, 15(2), 59–77. https://doi.org/10.1080/08821127.1998.10731971
- Brennen, B., & Brennen, J. S. (2015). Taking our pictures. Journalism Practice, 9, 520–535. https://doi.org/10.1080/17512786.2015.1030138
- Calvo, M. G., & Lang, P. J. (2004). Gaze patterns when looking at emotional pictures: Motivationally biased attention. Motivation and Emotion, 28(3), 221–243. https://doi.org/10.1023/B:MOEM.0000040153.26156.ed
- Caple, H., & Bednarek, M. (2016). Rethinking news values: What a discursive approach can tell us about the construction of news discourse and news photography. Journalism, 17(4), 435–455.
- Conley, D., & Lamble, S. (2006). The daily miracle: An introduction to journalism (3rd ed.). Oxford University Press.
- Costa, J. (1950). The complete book of press photography. The National Press Photographer’s Association.
- Craig, G. (1994). Press photographs and news values. Australian Studies in Journalism, 3, 182–200.
- Eastwood, J. D., Smilek, D., & Merikle, P. M. (2001). Differential attentional guidance by unattended faces expressing positive and negative emotion. Perception & Psychophysics, 63(6), 1004–1013. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03194519
- Elliott, D. (2003). Moral responsibilities and the power of pictures. In S. D. Ross & P. M. Lester (Eds.), Image’s that Injure: Pictorial stereotypes in the media (pp. 7–14). Bloomsbury.
- Elliott, D. (2011). Ethical responsibilities and the power of pictures. In Lester, P.M. (Ed.), Images that injure: Pictorial stereotypes in the media, 7–14.
- Emmett, A. S. (2011). Haunting photos: Differential perception and emotional response to the archetypes of news photography: A study of visual reception factored by gender and expertise. University of Maryland, College Park.
- Fahmy, S., Bock, M., & Wanta, W. (2014). Visual communication theory and research: A mass communication perspective. Springer.
- Fenske, M. J., & Eastwood, J. D. (2003). Modulation of focused attention by faces expressing emotion: Evidence from flanker tasks. Emotion, 3(4), 327. https://doi.org/10.1037/1528-3542.3.4.327
- Fenske, M. J., Raymond, J. E., & Kunar, M. A. (2004). The affective consequences of visual attention in preview search. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 11(6), 1055–1061. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03196736
- Fox, E., Russo, R., Bowles, R., & Dutton, K. (2001). Do threatening stimuli draw or hold visual attention in subclinical anxiety? Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 130(4), 681. https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-3445.130.4.681
- Frosh, P. (2001). Inside the image factory: Stock photography and cultural production. Media Culture & Society, 23(5), 625–646.
- Gade, P. J. (2011). Postmodernism, uncertainty, and journalism. In W. Lowrey & P. Gade (Eds.), Changing the news: The forces shaping journalism in uncertain times (pp. 63–82). Routledge.
- Galtung, J., & Ruge, M. (1965). ‘The structure of foreign news: The presentation of the Congo, Cuba and Cyprus crises in four Norwegian newspapers. Journal of Peace Research, 2(1), 64–90. https://doi.org/10.1177/002234336500200104
- Gaston, M. (n.d). Altered Pic Gets Pulitzer Prize-Winning Photographer Fired. Yummy Mummy Club | yummymummyclub.ca. Retrieved September 14, 2022, from https://www.yummymummyclub.ca/blogs/melissa-gaston-find-the-light/20140123/pulitzer-prize-winning-photographer-fired-for-altering(open in a new window)
- Gayle, G. (2018). The Perceived Credibility of Professional Photojournalism Compared to UserGenerated Content Among American News Media Audiences ( Order No. 28088107). Available from ProQuest Dissertations & Theses Global.
- Gaziano, C., & McGrath, K. (1986). Measuring the concept of credibility. The Journalism Quarterly, 63(3), 451–462. https://doi.org/10.1177/107769908606300301
- Gelpi, C., & Gartner, S. (2011). Affect and photos of success and failure in war. Paper presented at American Political Science Association 2011 Annual Meeting. https://ssrn.com/abstract=1900565(open in a new window)
- Godulla, A., Seibert, D., & Planer, R. (2021). Whose pictures, whose reality? Lines of tradition in the development of topics, negativity, and power in the photojournalistic competition world press photo. Journalism and Media, 2(4), 758–807. https://doi.org/10.3390/journalmedia2040045
- Gorn, G., Pham, M. T., & Sin, L. Y. (2001). When arousal influences ad evaluation and valence does not (and vice versa). Journal of Consumer Psychology, 11(1), 43–55. https://doi.org/10.1207/S15327663JCP1101_4
- Greenwood, K., & Thomas, R. J. (2015). Locating the journalism in citizen photojournalism: The use and content of citizen-generated photory. Digital Journalism, 3(4), 615–633. https://doi.org/10.1080/21670811.2015.1034528
- Greer, J. D., & Gosen, J. D. (2002). How much is too much? Assessing levels of digital alteration of factors in public perception of news media credibility. Visual Communication Quarterly, 9(3), 4–13.
- Horton, B. (1989). The picture: An associated press guide to good news photography. The Associated Press.
- Isaacowitz, D. M., Wadlinger, H. A., Goren, D., & Wilson, H. R. (2006). Selective preference in visual fixation away from negative photos in old age? An eye-tracking study. Psychology and Aging, 21(1), 40. https://doi.org/10.1037/0882-7974.21.1.40
- Kiousis, S. (2001). Public trust or mistrust? Perceptions of media credibility in the information age. Mass Communication & Society, 4(4), 381–403. https://doi.org/10.1207/S15327825MCS0404_4
- Klein-Avraham, I., & Reich, Z. (2016). Out of the frame: A longitudinal perspective on digitization and professional photojournalism. New Media & Society, 18(3), 429–446. https://doi.org/10.1177/1461444814545289
- Knobloch, S., Hastall, M., Zillmann, D., & Callison, C. (2003). Photory effects on the selective reading of internet newsmagazines. Communication Research, 30(1), 3–29. https://doi.org/10.1177/0093650202239023
- Kobré, K. (2011). Photojournalism: The professionals’ approach. (Ed. B. Brill, Vol. 5). Focal Press.
- Kovach, B., & Rosenstiel, T. (2021). The elements of journalism, revised and updated 4th edition: What newspeople should know and the public should expect. Crown Publishing Group.
- Lang, P. J., & Bradley, M. M. (2010). Emotion and the motivational brain. Biological Psychology, 84(3), 437–450. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsycho.2009.10.007
- Lang, P. J., Greenwald, M. K., Bradley, M. M., & Hamm, A. O. (1993). Looking at pictures: Affective, facial, visceral, and behavioral reactions. Psychophysiology, 30(3), 261–273.
- Lester, P. M. (2018). Visual ethics: A guide for photographers, journalists, and filmmakers. Taylor & Francis.
- Lester, P. M., Martin, S. A., & Smith-Rodden, M. (2022). Visual ethics: A guide for photographers, journalists, and media makers. Routledge.
- Lough, K. (2021). Judging photojournalism: The metajournalistic discourse of judges at the best of photojournalism and pictures of the Year contests. Journalism Studies, 22(3), 305–321. https://doi.org/10.1080/1461670X.2020.1867001
- Lowrey, W. (2002). Word people vs. picture people: Normative differences and strategies for control over work among newsroom subgroups. Mass Communication & Society, 5, 411–432. https://doi.org/10.1207/S15327825MCS0504_03
- Lydersen, K. (2013, July 8). Why a picture is worth a thousand words: laid-off photojournalists defend their field. In These Times. http://inthesetimes.com/working/entry/15251/laid_off_photojournalists_why_a_picture_is_worth_a_thousand_words(open in a new window)
- Machin, D. (2004). Building the world’s visual language: The increasing global importance of image banks in corporate media. Visual Communication, 3(3), 316–336.
- Marquart, F. (2023). Eye-tracking methodology in research on visual politics. In Research Handbook on Visual Politics (pp. 30–41). Edward Elgar Publishing.
- Martin-Kratzer, R. (2005). The emotional and cognitive processing of negative news photographs [ Doctoral Dissertation]. http://list.msu.edu/cgibin/wa?A2=ind0610d&L=aejmc&P=13361(open in a new window)
- Merrill, J. C. (1974). The imperative of freedom: A philosophy of journalistic autonomy. Freedom House.
- Metzger, M. J., Flanagin, A. J., Eyal, K., Lemus, D. R., & McCann, R. M. (2003). Credibility for the 21st century: Integrating perspectives on source, message, and media credibility in the contemporary media environment. Annals of the International Communication Association, 27(1), 293–335. https://doi.org/10.1080/23808985.2003.11679029
- Meyer, P. (1988). Defining and measuring credibility of newspapers: Developing an index. The Journalism Quarterly, 65(3), 567–574.
- Mortensen, T. M., McDermott, B. P., & Ejaz, K. (2023). Measuring photo credibility in journalistic contexts: Scale development and application to staff and stock photography. Journalism Practice, 17(6), 1158–1177.
- Müller, M. G., Kappas, A., & Olk, B. (2012). Perceiving press photography: A new integrative model, combining iconology with psychophysiological and eye-tracking methods. Visual Communication, 11(3), 307–328. https://doi.org/10.1177/1470357212446410
- Newton, J. (2013). The burden of visual truth: The role of photojournalism in mediating reality. Routledge.
- O’Rourke, C. (2022, March 25). No, these trees don’t prove White House tweeted an old photo of Joe Biden. Politifact. https://www.politifact.com/factchecks/2022/mar/25/viral-image/no-these-trees-dont-prove-white-house-tweeted-old-/(open in a new window)
- Perlmutter, D. D. (1998). Photojournalism and foreign policy: Icons of outrage in international crises. Praeger.
- Putterman, S. (2021, June 4). A New York Times photo error provoked social media criticism about a Gazan child. Poynter. https://www.poynter.org/fact-checking/2021/a-new-york-times-photo-error-provoked-social-media-criticism-about-a-gaza-child/(open in a new window)
- Raymond, J. E., Fenske, M. J., & Tavassoli, N. T. (2003). Selective attention determines emotional responses to novel visual stimuli. Psychological Science, 14(6), 537–542. https://doi.org/10.1046/j.0956-7976.2003.psci_1462.x
- Rimmer, T., & Weaver, D. (1987). Different questions, different answers? Media use and media credibility. The Journalism Quarterly, 64(1), 28–44.
- Rössler, P., Bomhoff, J., Haschke, J. F., Kersten, J., & Müller, R. (2011). Selection and impact of press photography. An empirical study on the basis of photo news factors. Communications, 36(4), 415–439. https://doi.org/10.1515/comm.2011.021
- Schwartz, D. (2020). Professional Oversight: Policing the Credibility of Photojournalism. In Image Ethics in the Digital Age (pp. 27–51). Minneapolis, MN: University of Minnesota.
- Self, C. C. (2014). Credibility. In An integrated approach to communication theory and research (pp. 449–470). Routledge.
- Shoemaker, P. J., & Reese, S. D. (2013). Mediating the message in the 21st century: A media sociology perspective. Routledge.
- Silva, M. F. S., & Eldridge II, S. (2020). The ethics of photojournalism in the digital age. Routledge.
- Simmonds, L., Bellman, S., Kennedy, R., Nenycz-Thiel, M., & Bogomolova, S. (2023). Moderating effects of prior brand usage on visual attention to video advertising and recall: An eye-tracking investigation. Journal of Business Research, 111, 241–248.
- Singer, J. B. (2007). Contested autonomy: Professional and popular claims on journalistic norms. Journalism Studies, 8(1), 79–95. https://doi.org/10.1080/14616700601056866
- Singletary, M. W., & Lamb, C. (1984). News values in award-winning photos. The Journalism Quarterly, 61(1), 104–233. https://doi.org/10.1177/107769908406100114
- Skinner, P. (2010). Sports photography: How to capture action and emotion. Skyhorse Publishing, Inc.
- Smith-Rodden, M., & Ash, I. K. (2012). Investigating the psychological effects of news photory: A case for evidence-based decision making and practices. Visual Communication Quarterly, 19(1), 20–32. https://doi.org/10.1080/15551393.2012.656056
- Smith-Rodden, M., & Ash, I. K. (2017). The effect of emotionally arousing negative photos on judgments about news stories. Visual Communication Quarterly, 24(1), 15–31. https://doi.org/10.1080/15551393.2016.1272418
- Sonderman, J. (2013, February 7). Use of generic photos can be dangerous for illustrating news stories. Poynter. Retrieved September 14, 2022, from https://www.poynter.org/reporting-editing/2013/use-of-generic-photos-to-illustrate-news-stories-can-be-dangerous/(open in a new window)
- Staff, P. (2018, February 12). Visuals have value and so do visual journalists. Poynter. https://www.poynter.org/reporting-editing/2018/visuals-have-value-and-so-do-visual-journalists/(open in a new window)
- Sundar, S. S., & Nass, C. (2001). Conceptualizing sources in online news. Journal of Communication, 51(1), 52–72.
- Tseng, S., & Fogg, B. J. (1999). Credibility and computing technology. Communications of the ACM, 42(5), 39–44.
- Wallon-Hárs.(2023). How to sell stock photos on alamy: Wahaviblog about stock photography. WahaviBlog. https://wahaviblog.com/en/sell-stock-photos-alamy(open in a new window)
- Wheeler, T., & Gleason, T. (1995). Photography or photofiction: An ethical protocol for the digital age. Visual Communication Quarterly, 2(1), 8–12.
- Zelizer, B. (1995). Journalism’s “last” stand: Wirephoto and the discourse of resistance. Journal of Communication, 45(2), 78–92. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1460-2466.1995.tb00729.x